Skip to content

What Experts Do and Don't Know

Humans around the world have knowledge about many things. Some of it is the kind one might write essays about, or learn about in graduate courses at university.

· 7 min read
What Experts Do and Don't Know

Some recent political events, from the result of the Brexit referendum to the nomination of Donald Trump as the Republican candidate in the US election, have been described in terms of clashes between the views of two tribes of people, often described as experts or elites and those who trust them on one side, and ordinary people on the other. While one may argue for one side or another on these specific issues, or have a preference for the views of experts or ordinary people in general, reflexively taking sides on this basis is neither principled nor likely to lead to correct judgments.

Personally, I have a Bachelor’s degree in physics and applied mathematics, a PhD in astrophysics, and an academic career in statistics. I know what it’s like to be an expert in a couple of small areas, and how easy it is to imagine myself as one in other areas. Many of my friends are smart academics, and usually fall on the expert side of the discourse. I understand their frustration and anger at, for example, anti-vaccination activists whose intellectually dishonest websites and anecdotal evidence are thought to outweigh the opinion of medical researchers in vaccine efficacy and safety. Similarly, my physics colleagues often receive unsolicited mail from people we might generously call amateur physicists, about how they have developed new theories of physics that are better than the mainstream ones. While we must leave the door ever-so-slightly ajar to this possibility, I am comfortable with the fact that most physicists don’t pay much attention to these ideas.

These experiences make it easy to see why “trust experts” is a good rule of thumb in many cases. However, to elevate this to a sacred principle misses the full picture. There are versions of the anti-expert story that are not necessarily naive, contain a great deal of truth, and may not be obvious.

First, let’s get the obvious out of the way. Clearly, being an expert in one field doesn’t guarantee expertise in another, a compelling example of this being Neil DeGrasse Tyson’s periodic naive tweets about biology or other non-physical sciences. It is wise not to expect perfection from public figures when they opine, and it is also wise to listen to biologists when they say he got something wrong.

Humans around the world have knowledge about many things. Some of it is the kind one might write essays about, or learn about in graduate courses at university. This is often technical information about a specialty, like you might encounter in engineering, or grand high-level thinking, as one might encounter in philosophy or sociology.

However, there is a much less exciting kind of knowledge that we also possess, though we don’t often think of it as knowledge. I know which particular foods are lacking in my fridge right now, and my wife is the only other person in the world who knows this. My aunt knows the route from her home to her workplace. My postman father knows where to direct his gaze in order to find the street numbers of businesses along a street. In his book Intellectuals and Society, American writer Thomas Sowell calls this “mundane knowledge” — the local, everyday facts that ordinary people know about their own circumstances, preferences, and the people and places most relevant to them. Obviously, the mundane knowledge of millions of people in a country adds up to quite a huge quantity of detailed and specific knowledge which no small group of experts could ever hope to learn.

Sowell urges his readers not to underestimate the value and impact of mundane knowledge. For example, while an expert in light bulbs might know why compact fluorescents are more energy efficient than the older incandescent bulbs, they do not know the financial circumstances of everyone in the community. For someone who needs to make their last $50 stretch until the next payday, replacing all their light bulbs right now is probably the wrong decision. This is part of why free market oriented economists are skeptical of decisions made by experts on behalf of ordinary people; while experts know a lot more about certain topics, they don’t know about the specific needs and preferences of each individual or household, and good decision making hinges on this information a lot of the time.

Another way experts can be wrong is if their field is disconnected from feedback mechanisms, so that an idea gains popularity because it resonates with experts for reasons other than empirical success. Clearly, the fact there are believing Christian theology experts, believing Islamic theology experts, and atheists who know all the arguments for their position, doesn’t mean we ought to “trust the experts” and become confused people who believe Jesus was the son of God, not the son of God, and that God doesn’t exist, all at the same time.

You don’t have to go as abstract as theology to see this process in action. Recently, New Zealand had two referenda about whether to adopt a new flag (in the end, we kept the current flag). The first referendum had us choose the most popular of five designs for what the new flag might be. The winner of this referendum was widely panned in the press as being ugly and violating the principles of flag design. One wonders what the principles of flag design are based upon. If it were how to design flags that appeal to most people, then the referendum result suggests those principles aren’t particularly reliable. If the criteria are aimed at something else, then the fact the citizens preferred a “bad design” should neither be surprising, nor reason to express dismay at the general population for not trusting flag experts. We voted for the flag we liked, not to score marks in a flag-designing exam.

flag

Perhaps more controversially, if you want an explanation for why there are more men than women in physics, you might ask people with a strong interest in the topic, who are very intelligent and read a lot about it. Most would explain the discrepancy in terms of sexist attitudes and/or unconscious bias in the field, and the risk of sexual harassment when one is a young woman entering a field containing a lot of men (especially men who might be lonely and/or socially awkward, if these specific stereotypes have any truth). If you ask an ordinary person on the street, they might say “more men than women like physics because it’s technical and about inanimate objects, and women prefer other subjects.” Sexist attitudes and harassment exist, as do differences in preferences. Yet in this case the experts, by favouring a certain category of explanation, are missing a large part of the truth that the average person might regard as obvious. (And in case it’s not clear, this is perfectly compatible with being welcoming to female physicists and opposing sexual harassment).

Climate change is another heated (pardon the pun) topic which often leads people to implore the populace to trust experts. We should indeed trust the experts on the basic facts of the area. As an outsider, the field appears to be following the procedures of science which are meant to (eventually) weed out incorrect ideas. One might worry about groupthink, or perhaps publication bias towards dire consequences as opposed to benign ones, but this is unlikely to cast doubt on the basic facts which have been known for decades.

However, we shouldn’t entrust policy matters to climate scientists, who are likely to prioritise their concerns over valid competing ones (like the doctors who called for the Rio Olympics to be cancelled due to the Zika virus, as though there would be no trade-offs). For example, when former Australian Prime Minister Tony Abbott said, “Coal is good for humanity,” he was widely lambasted as being a climate ignoramus, and I have a lot of sympathy for this conclusion. However, the inconvenient truth is that coal has been good for humanity, and will continue to
have uses for a while yet. This is compatible with there being good reasons to try to reduce our consumption of it. Australia’s carbon tax was also most likely good policy and should have been left in place. These three statements are consistent with each other, and can be found by trusting different categories of expert. However, many people favour one preferred kind of expert over another, saying, “Listen to climate scientists,” while ignoring inconvenient experts, such as coal power plant experts, who might point out that coal still is, in at least a couple of respects, better than renewable energy.

Overall, I think we should have a balanced relationship with experts. We should listen to them, but we should also find out how (or whether) they know what they know, and whether mundane knowledge is relevant in the specific circumstance being discussed. We should do this with a variety of experts, rather than elevating experts in one subject area above another in order to justify whatever preconceptions we bring to the discussion. We can and should take advantage of the insights of our most intelligent and well-informed people, without ever needing to become their subordinates.

Latest Podcast

Join the newsletter to receive the latest updates in your inbox.

Sponsored

On Instagram @quillette